Abstract: Human activity in Antarctica has increased sharply in recent years. In particular during the winter months, people are exposed to long periods of isolation and confinement and an extreme physical environment that poses risks to health, well-being and performance. The aim of the present study was to gain a better understanding of processes contributing to psychological resilience in this context. Specifically, the study examined how the use of coping strategies changed over time, and the extent to which changes coincided with alterations in mood and sleep. Two crews (N=27) spending approximately 10 months at the Concordia station completed the Utrecht Coping List, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS), and a structured sleep diary at regular intervals (x 9). The results showed that several variables reached a minimum value during the midwinter period, which corresponded to the third quarter of the expedition. The effect was particularly noticeable for coping strategies (i.e., active problem solving, palliative reactions, avoidance, and comforting cognitions). The pattern of results could indicate that participants during Antarctic over-wintering enter a state of psychological hibernation as a stress coping mechanism.
---
Discussion
The findings from this study suggest that coping
strategies, sleep quality, and PA were influenced by the environmental
conditions to a smaller or larger degree during midwinter. Quadratic
time-based models demonstrated the greatest effect sizes, suggesting
that when the conditions are harshest, resources are more depleted and
participants were less involved in any form of coping and reported less
PA. Also subjective sleep quality showed a negative trend over time, a
result consistent with other research (Bhargava et al., 2000; Pattyn et al., 2018)
although at the end of the stay the average score did increase
slightly. While chronic hypoxia might lead to deterioration in sleep
quality in high altitude (Collet et al., 2015), the effect of hypoxia on adaptation among residents on Concordia has been shown to persist over time (Porcelli et al., 2018).
Thus, we argue that hypoxia cannot explain seasonal variations in sleep
quality observed in this study. The reduction in sleep quality and PA
is consistent with the “midwinter syndrome” observed by other
researchers (Bhargava et al., 2000; Palinkas and Suedfeld, 2008).
It is noticeable that reports of NA remained low over time and did not
show the expected change during midwinter. One possible explanation is
that participants were reluctant to report distress.
Perhaps the most striking result from this study was the
reduction in all of observed coping strategies during the midwinter
period. This pattern contradicts the idea that emotional strategies and
avoidance take over from more active strategies in situations involving
chronic stressors. Our findings may reflect that participants became
more indifferent or emotionally flat during the winter months. This
interpretation is consistent with early research which noted the
occurrence of a mild psychological fugue state known as the Antarctic
stare, around the third quarter of the stay (Barabasz et al., 1983).
The phenomenon state is characterized by an altered state of
consciousness or pronounced absentmindedness, “drifting,” wandering off
attention, and deterioration in situational awareness. We believe that
this reaction is not unique to people overwintering in Antarctica. For
example, during a 520 days confinement study (MARS500) crew members
reported reduced need for stimulation around the third quarter (Sandal and Bye, 2015).
Interactions between crew members declined, and one crew member showed
indications of dissociation. The state of seeking reduced stimulation,
and emotional flatness bears resemblance to what could be called
“psychological hibernation.” A state of psychological hibernation may be
beneficial for coping with the harshness of prolonged exposure to
stress in extreme environments. The ability to “switch off” mentally has
been associated with positive outcomes in the work stress literature (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005).
However, psychological detachment has also been described as a symptom
of burnout after prolonged exposure to stress at work (Maslach and Leiter, 2016).
Whilst psychological hibernation could be an adaptive response to the
extreme conditions, especially if it disappears when conditions become
less extreme (as evidenced by the increase in coping strategy use
reported in the present study), understanding the nature of this
phenomenon should be an avenue for future research. Further research is
also needed to determine the extent to which this state might be
associated with decrement in cognitive function and the ability to react
to acute, safety-critical situations. So far studies on cognitive
performance investigations on Antarctica have been controversial. While
no detectable cognitive deterioration was found in a study of a crew
overwintering on Concordia (Barkaszi et al., 2016), other researchers have shown that residence in Antarctica had a detrimental effect on cognition (Reed et al., 2001).
No comments:
Post a Comment