Abstract: The article tests whether the personality of candidates – in terms of their Big Five (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and openness) and Dark triad (narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism) – is associated with their electoral results. Via a novel dataset based on expert ratings for 122 candidates having competed in 55 recent national elections worldwide, and controlling for several covariates, results show that a better performance at the ballot box is associated with high conscientiousness, openness to experience and psychopathy. Extraversion is negatively associated with better results. Analyses also reveal profile effects; extraversion is linked to worse results especially for incumbents and younger candidates, conscientiousness and narcissism are associated with better results especially for candidates on the right-hand side of the ideological spectrum, and openness is associated with better results for male candidates.
Keywords: electoral success; Big Five; Dark Triad; personality; candidates; comparative data
Main Results and Discussion
The electoral success of 122 candidates having competed in 55 elections worldwide
was regressed on their personality profile (plus controls). Several significant effects,
which resist most robustness checks, were found. A better performance at the ballot
box is associated with high conscientiousness, virtually on par with the “incumbency
bonus” and confirming a known trend in studies on business job (Salgado, 1997). Openness to experience is also linked positively with success (Joly et al., 2018) and so is psychopathy – again, in line with studies on job performance and business
(Babiak & Hare, 2006; Boddy et al., 2010). Extraversion is linked with lower success, perhaps because extroverted might be
perceived as lacking seriousness – thus, in line with what found for conscientiousness.
Results also reveal profile effects, that is, significant interactions between the
profile of candidates and their personality. Extraversion is linked with worse results
especially for incumbents and younger candidates, conscientiousness and narcissism
are linked with stronger success in candidates on the right-hand side of the ideological
spectrum, and openness is associated with greater success for men.
The rather strong effect of psychopathy might seem disturbing. After all, character
components often associated with this trait are high impulsivity, thrill-seeking,
low empathy and anxiety (Paulhus & Williams, 2002), definitely not ideal traits one could hope for in people we elect to lead us. Yet,
the results seem more likely to support the idea that “successful psychopaths” – that
is, individuals scoring high in non-clinical psychopathy which nonetheless show high
levels of extrinsic success, e.g., in their career – benefit from high conscientiousness
(Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2010). In the particularly antagonistic “social niche” of political competition, both
psychopathy and conscientiousness are linked with extrinsic success. The fact that
conscientiousness is the trait with virtually the strongest association with electoral
success, also considering other powerful alternatives, sheds a somewhat positive light
over electoral competition: if being likeable and nice counts less than being serious
and dependable, then the much discussed “Americanisation” of politics, where exchanges
about ideas are replaced by “beauty contests” where only the image matters, has not
yet achieved the dramatic levels some fear.
Limitations
This article describes the first cross-national large-scale comparative study of how
the (perceived) personality of candidates drives their electoral success. Yet, although
covering a large number of cases across virtually all regions of the globe, the representativeness
of the geographical coverage is contingent to the elections that took place in the
period under investigation. Data collection in the study is still under way, and future
iterations of the dataset will expand the scope – ideally towards full coverage of
the countries around the globe. A second limitation comes directly from the use of
expert judgments to measure the personality of elites. As discussed above, expert
ratings provide evaluations of perceived personality traits, and the question whether
these judgments reflect only a perceived public persona or, instead, are able to capture
differences in the elites’ personality structure is still up for grabs. Also contested
by some is whether external observers are able in the first place to provide an objective
assessment of public figures, especially in light of potential ideological biases
of “experts” (e.g. Wright & Tomlinson, 2018). Yet, evidence in several studies suggests that external observers are often able
to provide relatively unbiased estimations (e.g., Nai & Maier, 2019; Vazire, 2006); furthermore, from a normative standpoint the importance of ethical concerns such
as those expressed by the “Goldwater rule” are increasingly contested (e.g., Lilienfeld et al., 2018). Finally, the large-scale scope of the comparison made impossible the use of granular
data about support for different candidates at the voter level (e.g., by pairing the
dataset with representative mass post-electoral surveys in each country). Yet, reasons
exist to believe that the personality of elites has unique effects on different voters.
Voters tend to appreciate and support political figures with personalities that “match”
their own (Caprara & Zimbardo, 2004; Caprara et al., 2003; Caprara et al., 2007b; Fortunato et al., 2018), and certain candidate personality profiles might seem more appealing for some voters
but not for others – for instance the “drunken dinner guest” brash style of populists
(Arditi, 2007) could be particularly appreciated by voters scoring low on agreeableness (Bakker et al., 2016). Future research should strive to develop a better understanding of whether individual
differences moderate the effect of candidates’ personality in a comparative setting.
Conclusion: Theoretical, Methodological and Applied Implications
The research in the political consequences of elites’ personality is still in its
infancy. The few existing studies are either limited to specific traits (e.g., psychopathy;
Lilienfeld et al., 2012) or cases (e.g., Belgian elected officials; Joly et al., 2018). Yet, results presented in this article were globally in line with the evidence
discussed in those studies. From a theoretical perspective, the results contribute
to both the fields of political decision-making and behavioural consequences of individual
differences. First, the results suggest that contemporary accounts of electoral competition
are overlooking an important component – who the candidates are, beyond what they
propose and how they frame it – which seems likely to drive part of their electoral
fortune. In this sense, integrated models of voting choices should include, beyond
citizens’ attitudes and preferences, a more nuanced account of the “supply” side,
for instance in terms of candidates’ personality. Second, the results are consistent
with findings in other disciplines (e.g., business; Babiak & Hare, 2006; Hochwarter et al., 2000), suggesting that personality plays a similar role in different contexts.
From a methodological standpoint, because obtained with an alternative approach –
expert ratings – the results discussed here suggest that attention should be granted
in further research to initiatives that triangulate alternative approaches for the
measure of elite personality. Beyond comparative initiatives yielding self-ratings
of elites via standardized surveys, quite promising is the avenue of machine learning
automated coding of political speeches (Ramey et al., 2017, 2019), especially if coupled in the future with the automated coding (e.g., via computer
vision) of non-verbal cues – facial expression, voice pitch, bodily gestures. Forthcoming
research should strive to integrate alternative approaches towards a comprehensive
and multimodal understanding of personality as a performative act, where the textual
and lexical dimension of the communication is overlapped with the emotional behavior
of the individuals under investigation (Poria et al., 2017).
Finally, from an applied standpoint, the results are can be seen as new recipes to
be added to the cookbook of campaign consultants and spin doctors – or, at the very
least, as new variations of old recipes. Modern campaign consultants are naturally
drawn towards the use of more antagonistic communication techniques (e.g., Francia & Herrnson, 2007; Geer, 2012), seen as efficient tools to bolster candidates’ standings while keeping the opponents
at bay. This recipe seems consistent with the positive effects played by psychopathic
traits on electoral success, as described in the study. Yet, the fact that high extraversion
harms electoral performances while high conscientiousness enhances them is a call
for caution when designing excessively aggressive campaigns; if boldness is rewarded
in competitive social dynamics, candidates are still expected to perform their duties
seriously and in a competent manner. Recent studies have started exploring the link
between communication strategies and candidates’ personality traits (e.g., Nai et al., 2019), and abrasive political figures such as the 45th occupant of the White House will undoubtedly renew the attention of the discipline,
the media, and the public at large towards the role of personality and character in
politics.
No comments:
Post a Comment