When transgressors intend to cause harm: The empowering effects of revenge and forgiveness on victim well‐being. Peter Strelan Jan‐Willem Van Prooijen Mario Gollwitzer. British Journal of Social Psychology, December 11 2019. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12357
Abstract: When people are transgressed against, they are usually motivated to restore personal power that was threatened by the transgression. We argue and test the new idea that while revenge and forgiveness responses are typically seen as opposites, both may be empowering, depending on the offender’s intent to harm. Across two studies, one experimental (N = 381) and one employing an autobiographical recall paradigm (N = 251), we tested a moderated mediation model. Notably, we found that revenge is empowering at high levels of intent and forgiveness is empowering regardless of intent. Importantly, we also demonstrate that empowerment provides an explanation for the process by which getting revenge and forgiving are each associated with improved affective outcomes for victims.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Revenge
We found, as hypothesized, a significant revenge 9 intent interaction on empowerment
in both studies. In Study 1, when offender intent was high, taking revenge was less
disempowering than doing nothing (the control condition), and avengers experienced
more positive and less negative affect in that case. In Study 2, we observed a similar effect:
The more participants reported that perceived offender intent was high, the more
revenge was empowering. The differing methodological approaches that we employed
may account for the nuanced difference in the direction of effects between Study 1 and
Study 2. Study 1 used hypothetical scenarios, which (1) are comparably less emotionally
involving, and in which (2) people are probably more aware of the fact that, usually, in
Western societies revenge tends not to be socially acceptable (Yoshimura & Boon, 2018).
Study 2, however, used autobiographical stories, which were more involving and also less
prone to social desirability issues. Here, participants might have allowed themselves to
experience and/or report the empowering effects of taking revenge more strongly than in
Study 1. In any event, regardless of how revenge affected empowerment in the two
studies, our theoretical argument is sustained: When victims perceive that offenders
intended to cause harm, getting revenge is the more sensible tactic – compared to doing
nothing (Study 1) or not getting revenge (Study 2) – at least in terms of empowering
victims.
Compatibility with and extension of existing theorizing about revenge
Previous experimental research suggests revenge can be satisfying when victims can
see that their transgressor understands the reasons for revenge, or has learnt from it
(e.g., Funk et al., 2014; Gollwitzer et al., 2011), but revenge is likely to be unsatisfying
when it serves no clear function (e.g., Carlsmith & Darley 2008). Our findings fit with
the idea that taking revenge can make avengers feel both good and bad (see Eadeh
et al., 2017). Specifically, in Study 1, when intent was high, avengers felt more
empowered than participants who did nothing, so that avengers were more likely to
indicate positive affect. However, when empowerment was statistically controlled for,
taking revenge was negatively related to positive affect. This suggests that the hedonic
benefits of taking revenge can be explained by feelings of empowerment. These
findings are novel and add another piece to the puzzle regarding the hedonic qualities
of revenge.
In Study 2, the empowering effect of revenge on affective outcomes under conditions
of high intent was even more pronounced. In terms of direct effects, revenge was
associated with higher levels of negative affect and clinical symptoms. However, to the
extent that victims got revenge against deliberately hurtful transgressors, they felt
empowered, so much so that the relations with negative affect and clinical symptoms
flipped around: Getting revenge was now associated with less negative affect and fewer
clinical symptoms. Interestingly, revenge was also associated with higher levels of
positive affect, especially when offenders meant to hurt and avengers felt empowered.
In short, we provide further evidence that if revenge is indeed to be ‘sweet’, it needs to
be functional. As offender intent increases, revenge becomes an appropriate response
(e.g., McCullough et al., 2013). One function of revenge under conditions of high intent,
therefore, is that it serves to empower avengers, an experience which in turn helps make
revenge ‘sweet’. Finally, we have extended previous research. Earlier studies on the functionality of
revenge relied on an offender’s response for a victim to know if revenge was effective
(e.g., Funk et al., 2014; Gollwitzer et al., 2011). However, in the present studies, the
potential efficacy of revenge was under the avenger’s control (i.e., they only had to decide
if the transgression was intentional or not).
Forgiveness
In both studies, there was a main (direct) effect of forgiveness on empowerment, indicating
that forgiving is empowering. Notably, this relation was not qualified by an interaction with
intent, indicating that forgiveness is empowering regardless of the extent to which victims
perceive that offenders intend to cause harm. Furthermore, in Study 1, there was evidence
that forgiving helps victims feel less negative and more positive because it is, to some extent,
empowering. The direct effects on well-being are in line with a substantial literature
indicating the benefits of forgiving for victims (Cheadle & Toussaint, 2015; Griffin et al.,
2015; Larkin et al, 2015; McCullough, 2008; Witvliet & Luna, 2018). The indirect effect
through empowerment is new, providing initial evidence for the process by which
forgiveness leads to more positive and less negative affective outcomes.
In Study 2, forgiveness was positively associated with revenge and negative affect and
clinical symptoms – yet was also positively associated with empowerment and positive
affect. Although these relations seem incompatible, there are at least two interrelated
plausible explanations. One is that these findings reflect the reality of post-transgression
turmoil, wherein victims need to navigate conflicting response repertoires particularly in
good-quality relationships (as was the case in this study). For example, longitudinal
research shows that emotional responses oscillate in the aftermath of transgressions, so
that a person can indicate vengeful and benevolent motivations at the same time (e.g.,
McCullough et al., 2003). In addition, it is possible that the positive forgiveness–revenge
correlation reflects that participants had acted vengefully, which in turn enabled them to
forgive, consistent with research indicating that getting justice helps victims forgive (for a
review, see Strelan, 2018). The other explanation is methodological in nature. Wellestablished measures of forgiveness require participants to indicate their current
thoughts or feelings or motivations towards a transgressor, so that the classic forgiveness
versus revenge dichotomy emerges. That is, if a person is currently positively disposed
towards a transgressor, they cannot at the same time indicate that they are negatively
disposed towards them; therefore, the conflicting responses typically seen posttransgression are not captured. However, this was the first study in which victims’
perceptions of their forgiveness and revenge behaviours have been measured and
participants were asked to recall the extent to which they had acted vengefully or in a
forgiving manner. As a measure of recalling what one did, it allows a respondent to be
internally inconsistent, that is, to recall acting both positively and negatively towards a
transgressor.
When the empowering effect of forgiveness was taken into account (Study 2), the
significant positive relation between forgiveness and each of negative affect and clinical
symptoms disappeared, indicating a suppressor effect for empowerment. This suggests
that a sense of empowerment helps to render moot the positive relation between
forgiveness and those negative outcomes. Further, there was an indirect negative effect of
forgiveness on negative affect and clinical symptoms via empowerment. These relations
indicate that the less forgiving a person is, the less empowered they feel, and the more
negative their affective responses. In short, there is some evidence that if empowerment plays a role, it is to explain why lower levels of forgiveness may predict higher levels of
negative affect and clinical symptoms.
Compatibility with and extension of existing theorizing about forgiveness
Our findings speak to two aspects of the forgiveness literature. One is concerned with the
costs of forgiving, which effectively suggests that if offenders do not deserve forgiveness
(e.g., Strelan et al., 2016), then forgivers should experience forgiveness as costly. In the
present studies, offenders who intended harm would not deserve forgiveness, and
therefore, forgiving should prove to be a costly affective exercise for victims. Notably, in
Study 2, we found similar effects to Strelan et al. for the forgiveness 9 intent interaction
on the downstream variables: Forgiving was related to higher negative affect and lower
positive affect when intent was high – in other words, when offenders did not deserve
forgiveness (see footnote 2). However, in both our studies intent to harm did not affect the
extent to which forgivers felt empowered – in other words, even when offenders did not
deserve forgiveness, forgiveness was still empowering. These particular findings provide
further support for Strelan et al.’s theorizing that undeserved forgiveness is a costly
affective response, but they also suggest there may be nuances in the way undeserved
forgiving is experienced. To that end, our findings for empowerment are consistent with
another literature concerning the benefits of forgiveness, which suggests that forgiving is
empowering despite an offender’s bad behaviour and possibly even because of it (e.g.,
Enright & Fitzgibbons, 2000).
Wednesday, December 11, 2019
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment