Jacob Appleby, Christopher M. Federico, Joseph A. Vitriol, Allison L. Williams. 2020. “Infection and Ideology- Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Predicts Political Conservatism.” PsyArXiv. January 4. doi:10.31234/osf.io/6ag92
Abstract: Recent research on the behavioral immune system suggests that perceived vulnerability to disease is associated with greater ingroup preference, conformity, and support for established cultural practices. However, little of this research has looked at the implications of perceived vulnerability to disease for an orientation linked to many of the above outcomes: ideology. In two studies, we test the hypothesis that perceived vulnerability to disease should be associated with greater political conservatism. In Study 1, we find a relationship between perceived vulnerability to disease and increases in conservatism over time. In Study 2, we use data from the World Values Survey to demonstrate that perceived vulnerability to disease predicts a stronger preference for the political right in a large sample of respondents from a wide variety of nations. Together, these results suggest that the consequences of the behavioral immune system may extend to abstract identifications like ideology.
General Discussion
In these studies, we extend prior work on the social implications of the behavioral
immune system by examining the relation between subjective perceptions of disease
vulnerability and ideological self-placement. Consistent with the expectation that the perceived prevalence of pathogens activates motivational goals and values congruent with conservatism,
Study 1 found that perceived vulnerability to disease was associated with increases political
conservatism over time and a stronger tendency to evaluate conservatives more positively than
liberals net of baseline political beliefs. Extending this finding, Study 2 found that PVD was also
related to a greater preference for the political right in a large representative survey of
respondents from a wide variety of nations. In both cases, our results were robust to controls for
relevant demographic characteristics and competing psychological determinants of ideology.
Together, our findings address a major gap in our understanding of the behavioral
immune system’s social implications. Although PVD has been shown to predict conformity
(Murray & Schaller, 2012; Wu & Chang, 2012) and exclusionary outgroup attitudes (Faulkner et
al., 2004), the current studies are the first to provide direct evidence for a relationship between
PVD and ideological conservatism. Given the relatively abstract nature of ideology as an
identification (Jost et al., 2009), they suggest that the operation of the behavioral immune system
may also have consequences for outcomes less concrete and socially immediate than ingroup
bias or conformity. Methodologically, they provide a more precise look at the relationship
between the behavioral immune system and ideology than studies that rely solely on disgust
sensitivity as an indicator of pathogen avoidance, which have yielded inconsistent findings
(Tybur et al., 2009). By relying on a more direct indicator of pathogen avoidance that accounts
for both the subjective perceptions of susceptibility to infection and related affective responses,
our results help clarify the connection between the functioning of the behavioral immune system
and ideology.
Despite the strength of our evidence, our studies are not without their limitations. First,
our correlational data cannot provide decisive evidence for a causal link between PVD and ideology. That said, our inclusion of a lagged indicator for our dependent variable does allows us
to model change in conservatism across time as a function of PVD and overcome some of the
bias potentially introduced by feedback effects (Finkel, 1995). Second, in Study 2, we were only
able to use a simple proxy measure of PVD from the World Values Survey, rather than the
validated scale employed in Study 1(Duncan et al., 2009). Nevertheless, the WVS items measure
perceptions closely related to those included in the PVD scale, leaving us with confidence in
their face validity; the similarity of our results across the two studies (and two very different
samples) also testifies to the correspondence between the two measures.
The evidence we present for a relationship between PVD and conservatism also has a
number of broader implications, both for society as a whole and for future research. Like other
threats (e.g., Jost et al., 2003, 2009), increased attention to public health concerns associated with
infectious disease—both in communications from political elites and in media coverage—may
have the potential to produce a conservative shift in public opinion and ideological sympathies.
Such changes in the salience of disease threat may have both a main effect on conservatism, as
well as a moderating effect in which they activate individual differences in PVD and produce
especially large ideological shifts among those who feel chronically susceptible to infection.
Given the tendency for pathogen prevalence to produce especially strong reactions against
cultural outgroups and those who fail to conform to dominant social norms (e.g., Schaller et al.
2003; Thornhill & Fincher, 2012), we might also expect these effects to be especially strong
when disease threats are associated with “foreign” groups (e.g., West Africans in the case of
Ebola) or “non-normative” groups (e.g., gay men in the case of HIV). These questions await
future research.
Sunday, January 5, 2020
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment