Women’s sexual strategies in pregnancy. Jaclyn Magginetti, Elizabeth G. Pillsworth. Evolution and Human Behavior, Volume 41, Issue 1, January 2020, Pages 76-86, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.10.001
Abstract: Humans exhibit an unusual pattern of sexual behavior compared to other mammalian females. Women's extended sexuality has been hypothesized to be related to a variety of possible benefits, especially non-genetic reproductive benefits, such as securing male investment via reinforced pairbonds or paternity confusion. But sexual behavior also comes at a cost, particularly for pregnant women, in terms of energetic costs, potential disease, and possible harm to the fetus. We hypothesize, therefore, that sexual behavior in pregnant women should reflect adaptive strategies and that pregnant women will be particularly strategic about their sexual behavior in order to maximize potential benefits while minimizing potential costs. One hundred twelve pregnant women completed a survey of their partners' qualities and their sexual desires toward their primary partners and men other than their primary partners. Results showed that women's perceptions of relationship threat positively predicted sexual desire for primary partners, while their perceptions of their partner's investing qualities negatively predicted sexual desire for extra-pair mates. These qualities, as well as cues to partner's genetic quality and gestation age, also interacted in ways that suggest that pregnant women's sexual desires are sensitive to cues of future investment and relationship stability.
4. Discussion
The
results from this study provide much-needed insight into the strategic
use of sexual behavior in pregnant women. The data suggest that sexual
behavior in pregnancy reflects a strategic tool designed to benefit
women's reproductive success through a variety of means. In contrast to
previous literature that has reported a general decline in sexual desire
across pregnancy, and in contrast to some studies that have documented
apparent increases in desire specifically during the second trimester,
we found no evidence of any direct effect of gestational age on in-pair
sexual desire, either in a linear or curvilinear fashion. This suggests
that variation in sexual desire during pregnancy is neither a simple
hormonal response, nor likely to be a physiological byproduct of
pregnancy.
Based on existing primate literature, one
specific function that post-conception copulation may serve is to
confuse paternity, which may be beneficial either in the context of
potential risks posed by non-fathers to the offspring or if non-fathers
may be able to provide additional investment or resources to the child.
While misattributed paternity is only likely to occur if multiple mating
takes place relatively early in pregnancy, cultures that exhibit
beliefs in partible paternity provide an example of how sexual behavior
throughout pregnancy can impact the paternal investment and resources
available to a woman and her child (e.g., Walker et al., 2010).
In our data, we saw no evidence that extra-pair sexual attraction was
directly related to gestation age, but we did find that other factors
that were related to women's experience of extra-pair sexual attraction
tended to have greater effects earlier in pregnancy compared to later.
Many
lines of evidence have shown that even the suggestion of possible
infidelity can provoke extremely costly responses in male partners, from
relationship desertion to homicide (Buss & Shackelford, 1997; Buunk & Massar, 2019; Jewkes, 2002; Wilson & Daly, 1993).
Therefore, we expect that the potential benefits of pursuing any sort
of extra-pair strategy should only exceed the likely costs if the
benefits provided by the primary partner are low. Our data showed a
pattern consistent with this prediction, though statistically weak, in
which those women who rated their partners as less satisfactory in terms
of their investing qualities were more likely to also report
experiencing extra-pair sexual desire or flirtation, particularly early
in pregnancy. It is important to note that our composite variable of
extra-pair attraction included measures of both desire and potentially
observable behavior, specifically, flirting with extra-pair men. Even
subtle forms of overt behavior are likely to carry risks of partner
retribution or more general social sanctions, and thus we find in many
studies of human sexual strategies that measures of desire are more
useful in illuminating the underlying sexual psychology that ultimately
direct sexual behavior than are measures of the behaviors themselves
(e.g. Brtnicka, Weiss, & Zverina, 2009; Haselton & Gangestad, 2006; Larson, Pillsworth, & Haselton, 2012; Pillsworth & Haselton, 2006; Roney & Simmons, 2013).
In this study, too, we found that the predicted patterns were more
evident when excluding overt behaviors and looking only at the
unobservable factor of desire.
In addition to the
predicted effect of partner investing qualities on pregnant women's
extra-pair desires, we also observed a surprising effect of partner's
physical attractiveness. Studies of women's extra-pair sexual desires
across the ovulatory cycle have consistently shown that fertile women
are more likely to experience extra-pair sexual desire if their primary
partners are low in physical attractiveness (e.g. Pillsworth & Haselton, 2006). We observed the opposite effect among pregnant women; it was those with more
physically attractive partners who experienced greater extra-pair
sexual desire. This effect, like that of partner investing quality, was
most evident in early pregnancy, when paternity confusion is most likely
to occur. Previous research has suggested that men with greater cues to
genetic quality, such as physical attractiveness, are, on average, less
likely to be reliable investing long-term partners (Aitken, Lyons, & Jonason, 2013; Fisher, 2003).
Perhaps those women who have conceived with “cads” are more likely to
be seeking “dads” early in pregnancy. Although this is a striking
pattern, and theoretically consistent with our overall argument, we
caution against overinterpreting this finding. While statistically
significant, this pattern is less robust than the others observed in our
data, and could be susceptible to undue influence by outliers in the
data. We suggest that this is an area that warrants further research.
We
also predicted that an extra-pair strategy, whether with the motive of
confusing paternity, extracting resources from more than one male, or
mate-switching, should be predicted by perceived threats to the current
relationship. We did not, however, observe any evidence that perceived
threats predicted extra-pair attraction in our participants. Given that
relationship maintenance, discussed below, and extra-pair tactics are
ultimately opposing strategies, it seems likely that any attempt to
pursue both simultaneously could easily result in failure at both. Our
participant sample was heavily biased toward women in stable, committed
relationships who expressed very high satisfaction with their partners,
particularly in terms of their partners' investing qualities. A
relationship maintenance strategy is likely, on average, to be more
beneficial than a extra-pair strategy in this context, and may account
for our failure to find any effect of perceived relationship threats on
extra-pair attraction among this sample of pregnant women. Future
research would benefit from examining strategic variation within a wider
range of relationship contexts to uncover the contextual features that
will predict one strategy over the other.
The two final
strategies that have been proposed to explain post-conception
copulations in the primate literature are relationship maintenance (in
the context of “friendships,” “consortships,” or pairbonds) and
intrasexual competition. Both of these strategies focus on engaging the
primary partner (often, but not always, the father) in sexual
interactions. However, the cues that should evince the behavior are
likely to differ based on the strategy. If the aim is to secure future
resources from a cooperative male partner, then pregnant women's in-pair
sexual desire should best be predicted by both the value of that
investment and cues that such investment may be at risk. If, on the
other hand, the behavior is primarily intended to disrupt the
reproduction of other women, then it should be less sensitive to
features related to the male partner's likely investment and more
sensitive to cues of female competitors' reproductive status. Because we
did not have information in our data regarding the reproductive status,
including fertility, of female competitors, we can not completely
disambiguate these two strategies, but in general the data are
consistent with a relationship maintenance strategy.
In
our measure of perceived threats to the relationship, we distinguished
between “perceived poaching attempts” (perceptions that other women were
interested in or trying to “steal” one's partner) and “perceived
cheating” (perceptions that the primary partner was, in fact, engaging
in extra-pair sexual or romantic behavior). We found perceived
relationship threat was the single biggest predictor of pregnant women's
in-pair sexual desires, but only in the context of perceived poaching
attempts. Perceived partner cheating had no effect on any outcome
variables. As discussed above, however, our sample was heavily biased
toward women in stable and satisfying relationships, the vast majority
of whom reported no suspicions of partner cheating at all. We suggest
that in a more representative sample, we would likely find distinct
patterns based on the different types of threat, with perceived cheating
being more likely to evoke a mate-switching strategy, resulting in
increased extra-pair attraction. This should especially be the case if
the primary partner is perceived as a less reliable investor. It is also
possible that the increase in in-pair sexual desire in the context of
potential mate poaching might be better explained as an effect of mate
copying (Place, Todd, Penke, & Asendorpf, 2010)
than of relationship maintenance, and that women simply find their
partners more attractive when other women also find them desirable. In
our data, however, perceptions of poaching threat and partner cheating
had similarly negative relationships with women's satisfaction with
their partner's investing qualities and physical attractiveness, making
that explanation less likely. This is another area of research that
deserves more attention going forward.
In our data,
neither independent partner qualities, such as partner's physical
attractiveness or investing qualities, nor dyadic qualities, such as
sexual compatibility, had any statistically significant direct effects
on pregnant women's experience of in-pair sexual desire. These findings
suggest that it is the quality or stability of the relationship itself,
rather than the specific qualities of the partner, that is most
important to the pregnant woman in predicting her experience of sexual
desire for her partner. It is particularly informative that, controlling
for sexual compatibility, a partner's physical attractiveness was
unrelated to women's sexual desire for her partner. This strongly
suggests that sexual desire, in pregnant women as in fertile women,
follows a strategic logic and is not simply the result of being faced
with an attractive partner.
While there were no direct
effects of partner qualities on women's in-pair sexual desire, we did
find, as predicted, that the partner's qualities interacted with
perceived threats to the relationship to predict women's in-pair sexual
desire. A partner's willingness and ability to invest in a woman and her
children will define, in part, the cost of losing the relationship. And
in fact, we did observe a trend in the data suggesting that the effect
of perceived mate poaching attempts on in-pair sexual desire was
stronger among those women who rated their partners as better investors
compared to those who rated their partners as poorer investors (see Fig. 3).
This trend did not reach statistical significance, but we were, again,
limited by the small range of variation in women's ratings of their
partners' investing qualities. It is relevant to note here that factor
analysis indicated that among our sample of pregnant women, parenting
qualities (e.g., “good parenting,” “ability to be kind and
understanding,” and “desire for children”) and resource-related
qualities (e.g., “financial resources,” “social status,” and “ambition”)
all loaded onto a single factor that we interpreted as investing
qualities. In previous studies using a similar measure with non-pregnant
women, these two types of investment (parenting and resources) have
appeared to be separable to participants (e.g. Roberts, Craig Roberts, & Little, 2008).
This further indicates that the stability of the relationship, and the
cooperative parenting benefits that can be obtained therein, are likely
to be of particular importance to pregnant women.
Finally,
we also observed an unpredicted interaction between perceived poaching
attempts and gestational age on women's in-pair sexual desires. As
illustrated in Fig. 4,
this pattern demonstrates that the effect of perceived threats on
pregnant women's sexual desire is more pronounced among women who are
earlier in their pregnancies, relative to those who are further along.
While not specifically predicted, this pattern is consistent with the
general hypothesis that pregnant women may strategically employ sexual
behavior to secure or reinforce investment in their future offspring,
perhaps by reinforcing a partner's perception of paternity. A promising
direction for future research would be to investigate whether actual
likelihood of misattributed paternity or even a partner's unfounded
skepticism of paternity could influence the practice and timing of
sexual behavior in response to perceived threats to the stability of the
relationship.
The current study has some limitations,
chief among them the relatively small and largely homogenous sample
population. Despite recruiting online and receiving responses from all
over the U.S., the sample was largely Caucasian, middle-class, and
more-or-less happily married. To more fully investigate the strategic
patterns of women's sexuality in pregnancy, it will be necessary to have
a more diverse sample in terms of relationship status, resource access,
and social support. Another limitation was that our sample lacked women
very early in pregnancy. 95% of our sample were at least a month into
their pregnancies, and while we think it is unlikely that this
negatively affected the ability to test our hypotheses, future studies
would benefit from recruiting women trying to conceive and those in the
post-partum period in order to test predictions from the time of
conception through the first weeks of parenthood.
No comments:
Post a Comment