From 2019... The future-directed functions of the imagination: From prediction to metaforesight. Adam Bulley, Jonathan Redshaw, Thomas Suddendorf. In book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination, Apr 2019. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/332154143
Rolf Degen's take: https://twitter.com/DegenRolf/status/1310592701960777728
Abstract: One of the fundamental roles of human imagination is to enable the representation of possible future events. Here, we survey some of the most critical abilities that this foresight supports: anticipating future emotions, setting and pursuing goals, preparing for threats, deliberately acquiring skills and knowledge, and intentionally shaping the future environment. Furthermore, we outline how metacognition bolsters human capacities even further by enabling people to reflect on and compensate for the natural limits of their foresight. For example, humans make contingency plans because they appreciate that their initial predictions may turn out to be wrong. We suggest that the processes involved in monitoring, controlling, and ultimately augmenting future-oriented imagination represent an important and understudied parallel of "metamemory" that should be called "metaforesight".
2. Compensating for anticipated limits: introducing “metaforesight”
Humans, perhaps uniquely, are capable of meta-representational insight into
the relationship between their imagination and reality. In other words, people can
evaluate how imagined scenarios link in with the external world, and thus assess
whether what is imagined is likely to actually occur in the future, and whether it is
biased, pessimistic, or hopeful and so forth. In the broad sense, meta-representation
involves representing the relation between (i) a representation and (ii) what that
representation is about (Pylyshyn, 1978). The development of such a capacity in
childhood is widely considered as critical to the emergence of an understanding of
other people’s minds (e.g., Perner, 1991). In the domain of foresight, this form of
metacognition has long been given a central role (Suddendorf, 1999). Once one
appreciates that one’s thoughts about the future are just representations, one is in a
position to evaluate them, to modify them, to discount them, to discuss them, and to
try to compensate for their shortcomings (Redshaw, 2014; Redshaw & Bulley, 2018).
Indeed, this capacity may be crucial to children acquiring a mature sense of future
time itself – as a series of possible chains of events of which only one will actually
happen (see Hoerl & McCormack, 2018).
No comments:
Post a Comment