Pescetelli, Niccolo, Alexis Rutherford, and Iyad Rahwan. 2020. “Diversity Promotes Collective Intelligence in Large Groups but Harms Small Ones.” PsyArXiv. January 2. doi:10.31234/osf.io/b8q2c
Abstract: Diverse groups are often said to be less susceptible to decision errors resulting from herding and polarization. Thus, the fact that many modern interactions happen in a digital world, where filter bubbles and homophily bring people together, is an alarming yet poorly understood phenomenon. But online interactions are also characterized by unprecedented scale, where thousands of individuals can exchange ideas simultaneously. Evidence in collective intelligence however suggests that small (rather than large) groups tend to do better in complex information environments. Here, we adopt the well-established framework of social learning theory (from the fields of ecology and cultural evolution) to explore the causal link between diversity and performance as a function of group size. In this pre-registered study, we experimentally manipulate both group diversity and group size, and measure individual and group performance in realistic geo-political judgements. We find that diversity hinders the performance of individuals in small groups, but improves it in large groups. Furthermore, aggregating opinions of modular crowds composed of small independent but homogeneous groups achieves better results than using non-modular diverse ones. The results are explained by greater conflict of opinion in diverse groups, which negatively impacts small (but not large) groups. The present work sheds light on the causal mechanisms underlying the success (or lack thereof) of diverse groups in digital environments, and suggests that diversity research can benefit from adopting a wider social learning perspective.
Full text downloadable at https://psyarxiv.com/b8q2c/download (PDF)
Thursday, January 2, 2020
Women: Beliefs in one’s sexual power lead both to negative (via self‐objectification, eating less) and positive (via sexual subjectivity, more sexual desire and pleasure) consequences for mental health
Sex is power belief and women’s mental health: The mediating roles of self‐objectification and sexual subjectivity. Matthias De Wilde, Annalisa Casini, Robin Wollast, Stéphanie Demoulin. European Journal of Social Psychology, November 12 2019. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2643
Abstract: Sex is power belief (SIPB) positively relates to self‐objectification. This research aims at expanding this finding. We propose that SIPB involves an instrumental view of one’s own body (i.e., self‐objectification) that leads women to experience the negative consequences classically associated with self‐objectification. We further suggest that SIPB positively relates to sexual subjectivity—multidimensional sexual self‐perceptions and positive sexual experiences—and that such relation counterbalances some of the negative effects of SIPB. We examine the effect of SIPB on women’s negative eating attitudes and sexual satisfaction, and test the mediating roles of self‐objectification and sexual subjectivity in three studies (N1 = 121, N2 = 296, N3 = 320). Results supported our predictions that beliefs in one’s sexual power lead both to negative (via self‐objectification) and positive (via sexual subjectivity) consequences for women’s mental health. The discussion focuses on the potential consequences of SIPB at both individual and collective levels.
General Discussion
The overall aim of the present research was to provide a more complete and nuanced
picture of the relationship between SIPB and SO. More precisely, across three studies, we
aimed to examine the consequences of SIPB on women’s health (i.e., negative eating
attitudes and sexual satisfaction) and to assess two possible underlying mechanisms of these
relationships (i.e., SO and sexual subjectivity).
Results of studies 1, 2 and 3, indicated that SIPB was positively related to SO.
Women who perceive that their body is a source of power are more prone to focus on the
observable part of themselves and to consider it as important for their self-concept (i.e., selfobjectify).
These results are in line with the idea that some women consider that matching
beauty and thinness standards is a stairway to power over men (Erchull & Liss, 2013b, 2014).
Indeed, media in contemporary occidental society teach women from an early age that
sexualized behaviors are rewarded (Murnen & Smolak, 2012) and that they can – or even
should – feel empowered when expressing and displaying their sexuality (Erchull & Liss,
2013ab; Tolman, 2012). In this sense, these results replicated prior findings from Erchull and
Liss (2013a) which reported a positive correlation between SIPB and SO. In addition, our
results are in line with prior studies that reported a positive relationship between variables
conceptually close to SIPB (e.g., beauty as currency and enjoyment of sexualization) and SO
(Calogero, et al., 2017; Erchull & Liss, 2014).
Moreover, in line with the literature on SO (Moradi & Huang, 2008; Roberts et al.,
2018), results of studies 1, 2, and 3 showed that the positive relationship between SIPB and
SO is related to deleterious consequences for women’s health, such as increasing women’s
negative eating attitudes and decreasing women’s sexual satisfaction (considered as a health
indicator; Higgins, Mullinax, Trussell, Davidson, & Moore, 2011). This result legitimates the
concern of scholars about the illusory nature of feeling a sense of power using their body
(Calogero & Siegel, 2018; Gill, 2008, 2012; Liss et al., 2010; Anderson, 2014). Accordingly,
our results indicate that SIPB is related to women’s attempt to comply with men’s
expectations (e.g., negative eating attitudes). Further, some scholars claimed that women who
experience a sense of power through the use of their body have actually internalized
extremely deeply the objectifying perspective of society (e.g., Gill, 2012) and that this source
of power is implicitly reserved to women who best fit men’s expectations (Gill, 2008). Our
findings support scholars’ concerns with the fact that women’s desire, pleasure, and
subjectivity could be devalued by the message that their sense of power is, in fact, a “false
consciousness marketed to them by a sexualized advertising culture” (Lamb & Peterson,
2012, p. 705). Taken together, these results converge with the idea that, subjectively
empowering or not, SO remains deleterious for women’s health and sexual functioning.
In addition, on top of the deleterious consequences SIPB has via SO, results of
Studies 2 and 3 also indicated that SIPB positively relates to sexual satisfaction via an
increase in sexual subjectivity. Consistent with Peterson’s claim (2010), our results show that
beliefs in one’s sexual power increase women sense of empowerment, their subjective right
to feel attractive and sexually desirable (Horne & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2006), their perception
of efficacy and entitlement to sexual desire and pleasure (Tolman, 2002, 2012), and their
sexual self-reflection (Zimmer-Gembeck & French, 2016). Because increased sexual
subjectivity relates positively to one’s sexual satisfaction (Horne & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2006;
Zimmer-Gembeck & French, 2016), this translates into a positive relationship between SIPB
and sexual satisfaction and an overriding of the deleterious consequences SIPB has on sexual
satisfaction via an increase in SO. Interestingly, additional analyses reported in studies 2 and
3 seem to underline the important driving role of two sub-components of the sexual
subjectivity construct in the relationship between SIPB and sexual satisfaction, i.e., sexual
body esteem and perceived sexual efficacy. Future research should be conducted to further
replicate this unexpected results, and to provide a better understanding of these relationships.
Abstract: Sex is power belief (SIPB) positively relates to self‐objectification. This research aims at expanding this finding. We propose that SIPB involves an instrumental view of one’s own body (i.e., self‐objectification) that leads women to experience the negative consequences classically associated with self‐objectification. We further suggest that SIPB positively relates to sexual subjectivity—multidimensional sexual self‐perceptions and positive sexual experiences—and that such relation counterbalances some of the negative effects of SIPB. We examine the effect of SIPB on women’s negative eating attitudes and sexual satisfaction, and test the mediating roles of self‐objectification and sexual subjectivity in three studies (N1 = 121, N2 = 296, N3 = 320). Results supported our predictions that beliefs in one’s sexual power lead both to negative (via self‐objectification) and positive (via sexual subjectivity) consequences for women’s mental health. The discussion focuses on the potential consequences of SIPB at both individual and collective levels.
General Discussion
The overall aim of the present research was to provide a more complete and nuanced
picture of the relationship between SIPB and SO. More precisely, across three studies, we
aimed to examine the consequences of SIPB on women’s health (i.e., negative eating
attitudes and sexual satisfaction) and to assess two possible underlying mechanisms of these
relationships (i.e., SO and sexual subjectivity).
Results of studies 1, 2 and 3, indicated that SIPB was positively related to SO.
Women who perceive that their body is a source of power are more prone to focus on the
observable part of themselves and to consider it as important for their self-concept (i.e., selfobjectify).
These results are in line with the idea that some women consider that matching
beauty and thinness standards is a stairway to power over men (Erchull & Liss, 2013b, 2014).
Indeed, media in contemporary occidental society teach women from an early age that
sexualized behaviors are rewarded (Murnen & Smolak, 2012) and that they can – or even
should – feel empowered when expressing and displaying their sexuality (Erchull & Liss,
2013ab; Tolman, 2012). In this sense, these results replicated prior findings from Erchull and
Liss (2013a) which reported a positive correlation between SIPB and SO. In addition, our
results are in line with prior studies that reported a positive relationship between variables
conceptually close to SIPB (e.g., beauty as currency and enjoyment of sexualization) and SO
(Calogero, et al., 2017; Erchull & Liss, 2014).
Moreover, in line with the literature on SO (Moradi & Huang, 2008; Roberts et al.,
2018), results of studies 1, 2, and 3 showed that the positive relationship between SIPB and
SO is related to deleterious consequences for women’s health, such as increasing women’s
negative eating attitudes and decreasing women’s sexual satisfaction (considered as a health
indicator; Higgins, Mullinax, Trussell, Davidson, & Moore, 2011). This result legitimates the
concern of scholars about the illusory nature of feeling a sense of power using their body
(Calogero & Siegel, 2018; Gill, 2008, 2012; Liss et al., 2010; Anderson, 2014). Accordingly,
our results indicate that SIPB is related to women’s attempt to comply with men’s
expectations (e.g., negative eating attitudes). Further, some scholars claimed that women who
experience a sense of power through the use of their body have actually internalized
extremely deeply the objectifying perspective of society (e.g., Gill, 2012) and that this source
of power is implicitly reserved to women who best fit men’s expectations (Gill, 2008). Our
findings support scholars’ concerns with the fact that women’s desire, pleasure, and
subjectivity could be devalued by the message that their sense of power is, in fact, a “false
consciousness marketed to them by a sexualized advertising culture” (Lamb & Peterson,
2012, p. 705). Taken together, these results converge with the idea that, subjectively
empowering or not, SO remains deleterious for women’s health and sexual functioning.
In addition, on top of the deleterious consequences SIPB has via SO, results of
Studies 2 and 3 also indicated that SIPB positively relates to sexual satisfaction via an
increase in sexual subjectivity. Consistent with Peterson’s claim (2010), our results show that
beliefs in one’s sexual power increase women sense of empowerment, their subjective right
to feel attractive and sexually desirable (Horne & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2006), their perception
of efficacy and entitlement to sexual desire and pleasure (Tolman, 2002, 2012), and their
sexual self-reflection (Zimmer-Gembeck & French, 2016). Because increased sexual
subjectivity relates positively to one’s sexual satisfaction (Horne & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2006;
Zimmer-Gembeck & French, 2016), this translates into a positive relationship between SIPB
and sexual satisfaction and an overriding of the deleterious consequences SIPB has on sexual
satisfaction via an increase in SO. Interestingly, additional analyses reported in studies 2 and
3 seem to underline the important driving role of two sub-components of the sexual
subjectivity construct in the relationship between SIPB and sexual satisfaction, i.e., sexual
body esteem and perceived sexual efficacy. Future research should be conducted to further
replicate this unexpected results, and to provide a better understanding of these relationships.
Prosocials were more optimistic about the future of morally good (than bad) agents, while individualists were not; this was driven by prosocials’ failure to update beliefs from undesirable information about morally good agents
Zheng, Shuying, Xinyuan Yan, Jenifer Siegel, Vladimir Chituc, Shiyi Li, Molly Crockett, and Yina Ma. 2020. “Self-serving Karmic Beliefs: Prosociality Influences Vicarious Optimism.” PsyArXiv. January 2. doi:10.31234/osf.io/ecqgf
Abstract: Belief in karma is ubiquitous, appearing early in development and impacting prosocial behavior. Here, we tested the possibility that karmic beliefs are self-serving: are “good” people more likely to believe that good things happen to good people? Study 1 (n=170) showed stronger karmic beliefs in more prosocial individuals. Next, we tested whether self-serving karmic beliefs arose from a motivated deployment of vicarious optimism: prosocial individuals adopt karmic beliefs by prioritizing desirable (the fortunes of good people, the misfortunes of bad people) over undesirable information when predicting the future. Study 2 (n=107) showed that prosocials were more optimistic about the future of morally good (than bad) agents, while individualists were not. This was driven by prosocials’ failure to update beliefs from undesirable information about morally good agents. Together, we suggest that karmic beliefs are self-serving, and result from a failure to update beliefs from information that conflicts with a karmic worldview.
Discussion
Karma
denotes the belief that good things will happen to people who have done good
deeds, while misfortunes will befall bad people in the future. In the current
studies, combining the moral character learning and vicarious belief update
tasks, we are able to quantify the beliefs about the future of people who have
done objectively good or bad deeds. We show that
individuals hold optimistic beliefs about the future of good people and
discount undesirable feedback when predicting their futures. In contrast,
individuals similarly incorporate desirable and undesirable feedback into their
beliefs about bad people’s futures. These results suggest that vicarious
optimism is one possible cognitive mechanism that gives rise to karmic beliefs.
Furthermore, we show that prosocial individuals (relative to individualists)
hold stronger karmic beliefs and stronger vicarious optimism for good relative
to bad people, suggesting that karmic beliefs are self-serving: good people
more likely to believe that good things happen to good people.
People
hold karmic beliefs in both first-party and third-party contexts (Hafer &
Olson, 1989). If prosocials and individualists hold karmic beliefs to a similar
extent, we might expect strong optimistic belief updating for the self in
prosocials, and pessimistic belief updating for the self in individualists.
However, we observed that prosocials and individualists were similarly
optimistic about their own futures. One potential explanation is that the
wishful thinking for oneself outweighs karmic believes when there are any
conflicts (Mata & Simão, 2019). Alternatively, individualists may not identify
themselves as “bad people” given vast evidence that most people tend to view
themselves in a positive light (Sanitioso & Wlodarski, 2004). Thus, it is
possible that individualists believe in karma but view themselves as good
people who deserve an optimistic future. Given that we found weaker karmic
beliefs in individualists, the finding of similar optimistic beliefs for the
self in individualists and prosocials would lend further support to our
hypothesis that karmic beliefs are self-serving, so that strong karmic beliefs
motivates prosocial individuals to believe in a bright future (possibly caused
by the good deeds they did). Taken together, this suggests that the
self-serving nature of karmic beliefs applies to both the self and other
people.
One
limitation of our second study is that we only provide evidence for ‘half’ of
the karmic worldview, i.e., that good things will happen to good people; we did
not observe evidence for beliefs that bad things will happen to bad people.
Even prosocials who showed stronger karmic beliefs did not express pessimistic
beliefs about bad agents. This might be due to that, in
prosocials’ karmic belief system, good things not happening is already a type
of punishment for the bad people, given that prosocial generally care about
others and prefer not to do harm to others (Penner., Dovidio., Piliavin., &
Schroeder., 2005), thus prosocials do not
predict bad consequence for morally bad people. Indeed, the current sample, we
found evidence that prosocials showed stronger harm aversion in the moral
decision task where they trade off profit for themselves against pain for another
person (harm aversion: prosocials vs. individualists, t(101) = 4.39, p <
0.001, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.29], Cohen d’ = 0.43).
In conclusion, the current study provides a novel
framework to decipher the cognitive processes that give rise to karmic beliefs,
and further proposes that karmic beliefs may be subject to self-serving
motivations. Our findings suggest that karmic beliefs – a feature of many
religious traditions – may be a key component of a positive feedback loop
between beliefs and behavior that together contribute to large-scale
cooperation.
Sense of coherence is a health resource that moderates stress and helps limit the occurrence of overweightness and eating disorders
The Relationship between Sense of Coherence, Stress, Body Image Satisfaction and Eating Behavior in Japanese and Austrian Students. Yoshiko Kato et al. Psych 2019, 1(1), 504-514, November 14 2019. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych1010039
Abstract: Background: Restrained, emotional, and external eating are related to obesity and eating disorders. A salutogenic model has confirmed sense of coherence (SOC) as a health resource that moderates stress and helps limit the occurrence of overweightness and eating disorders. This study aimed to examine the relationship between SOC, social support, stress, body image satisfaction (BIS) and eating behaviors in different cultural environments. Methods: A total of 371 Austrian (161 men, 210 women) and 398 Japanese (226 men, 172 women) university students participated. The SOC-13 scale, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, BMI-Based Silhouette Matching Test and an analogue single-stress item were used as measurements. Results: SOC negatively affected all three types of eating in Austrian students (men: β = −0.227 to −0.215; women: β = −0.262 to −0.214). In Japanese students, SOC negatively affected external eating in both sexes (men: β = −0.150; women: β = −0.198) and emotional eating (β = −0.187) in men. BIS indicated that the desire to become slim predicted restrained eating, women’s emotional eating, and men’s and Austrian women’s external eating. Stress was only predictive of emotional eating in Japanese men. Conclusions: This study found that SOC, BIS and stress might be valuable factors regulating eating behavior in a cultural context. However, the relationship between SOC, BIS, stress and eating behavior differs between cultures.
Keywords: sense of coherence; restrained eating; emotional eating; external eating; body image satisfaction; cross-culture
---
emotional eating (EME), external eating (EXE), restrained eating (RE)
Abstract: Background: Restrained, emotional, and external eating are related to obesity and eating disorders. A salutogenic model has confirmed sense of coherence (SOC) as a health resource that moderates stress and helps limit the occurrence of overweightness and eating disorders. This study aimed to examine the relationship between SOC, social support, stress, body image satisfaction (BIS) and eating behaviors in different cultural environments. Methods: A total of 371 Austrian (161 men, 210 women) and 398 Japanese (226 men, 172 women) university students participated. The SOC-13 scale, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, BMI-Based Silhouette Matching Test and an analogue single-stress item were used as measurements. Results: SOC negatively affected all three types of eating in Austrian students (men: β = −0.227 to −0.215; women: β = −0.262 to −0.214). In Japanese students, SOC negatively affected external eating in both sexes (men: β = −0.150; women: β = −0.198) and emotional eating (β = −0.187) in men. BIS indicated that the desire to become slim predicted restrained eating, women’s emotional eating, and men’s and Austrian women’s external eating. Stress was only predictive of emotional eating in Japanese men. Conclusions: This study found that SOC, BIS and stress might be valuable factors regulating eating behavior in a cultural context. However, the relationship between SOC, BIS, stress and eating behavior differs between cultures.
Keywords: sense of coherence; restrained eating; emotional eating; external eating; body image satisfaction; cross-culture
---
emotional eating (EME), external eating (EXE), restrained eating (RE)
4. Discussion
The
main findings of this study indicate that SOC and BIS pertain
negatively to RE, EME, and EXE with the appearance of culture and gender
differences. In particular, SOC negatively predicts RE, EME, and EXE,
particularly in Austrian students. For Japanese students, the
relationship between SOC and eating behaviors seems to be less
pronounced.
Japanese students had higher scores
for RE and EXE but did not differ in EME. Not all eating behaviors
reflect disordered eating, per se, but some eating behaviors seem to be
associated with occasional overeating and moderate overweightness, such
as restrained and emotional eating [31]. High RE values do not differentiate between successful and unsuccessful restrained eaters [7,32].
Considering the higher percentage of underweight Japanese students, RE
seems to be a highly and successfully practiced eating behavior in
Japan, independent of SOC and social support. Only BIS predicts RE in
Japanese students, whereas SOC seems to be a source of eating
regulation, in addition to BIS, in Austrian students. However,
considering the higher rate of overweight students in Austria, this
regulation is less successful for Austrian students than for Japanese
students. SOC predicted EME in both male and female Austrian students
and in Japanese male students, but not in Japanese female students. In
the Japanese student sample, BIS was a significant predictor in women
but not men, and stress was another predictor of EME. EXE was related to
SOC in both countries, and BIS helped predict EXE in Austrian men and
women and Japanese men but not in Japanese women. Stress correlated
negatively and SOC correlated positively with social support among
Japanese women; however, both variables disappeared as predictors of
eating behavior in Japanese women, and seemed to be coping resources.
A
previous study with Dutch subjects found that being female and having a
strong SOC, a flexible RE, and self-efficacy promoted healthy eating
practices [33].
The work of Speirs et al. suggested that a higher SOC is expected to
prevent unhealthy eating practices and foster healthy eating behaviors
in children [14]. This study found that SOC tended to prevent unhealthy eating behaviors.
In
this investigation of two cultures, we examined the association between
SOC and eating behaviors, based on a salutogenic model. SOC indicates
the extent to which an individual has a pervasive and enduring, yet
dynamic, feeling of confidence that the environment is predictable and
that things will work out as well as can reasonably be expected. SOC
contains aspects of optimism and control and represents the ability to
cope with stressful events and find them meaningful [15]. A previous study reported that a strong SOC may confer some resilience against chronic diseases [34].
The results of the present study suggest that a high SOC might prevent
RE, EME, and EXE in Austrian subjects. In contrast, SOC affected EXE in
Japanese men and women and EME in Japanese men. SOC and stress were
strongly associated in Japanese students and had a more indirect
association with eating behaviors. In Austrian students, SOC, stress and
social support were weakly correlated, while SOC was more strongly
correlated with eating behaviors.
A higher SOC has been reported to relate to less perceived stress and a lower stress response [35]. Some studies have reported that obesity and eating disorders are caused by stressful situations [36].
Controlling stress is therefore necessary to maintain healthy eating
behaviors. Because the degree of stress affected EME in Japanese men in
the present study, we suggest that improving SOC might be effective in
reducing EME via stress reduction. Social support, as an external health
factor resource, was not directly related to the three eating behaviors
but was correlated with SOC in this study. Social support seems to be
associated with eating behaviors via SOC-mediated effects. This finding
is in line with the results of our previous study, which showed that
eating behavior was associated with interpersonal relationships [37].
Overall,
as expected, the results supported the hypothesis that SOC affects
eating behaviors and overweight-related eating behaviors more directly
in Austrian students, whereas SOC relates to stress reduction in
Japanese students and indirectly relates to eating habits.
It
is important to note that cultural and gender-based differences were
observed between variables in this study. The obesity rate was lower in
women than in men, and the desire to become slim and RE were more
prevalent in women than in men. Ideal body images are affected by
cultural expectations, which often lead women to want to become thinner.
Such a cultural context fosters restrained eating behaviors in women,
and the degree to which women desire to become slimmer predicts RE.
Conversely, the structure of the relationship between the desire to get
slimmer and EME or EXE may differ by country and gender. In women, the
desire to be slim was positively related to EME, while in Austrian and
Japanese men, the desire to be slim was related to EXE. This result is
in line with theories related to the DEBQ. Both EXE and EME are regarded
as consequences of intense RE. Small positive differences between ideal
and real body images were found among Japanese men, because young men
are mainly concerned with gaining muscle rather than becoming slim [38].
Our results show that the highest rate of thinness was in Japanese
women, and the desire to become slimmer might control EXE. These results
support our previous findings [22].
A
strength of this study is that it investigated a homogeneous group; all
participants were university students. Additionally, all variables were
investigated in their respective cultural groups using the same
methodology in both Western and Asian countries [39].
However,
this study had some limitations. First, we used one analogue scale to
measure the degree of stress, to avoid a severe burden on the
participants. A more sophisticated measure of stress should be used in
future studies. A negative relationship was found between stress and
resilience in Japanese students. In Austrian students, this negative
relationship was weak and not significant. In Austria, the validity of
the stress scale is insufficient. In the future, it is necessary to use a
more validated stress scale and examine the relationship with eating
behavior. Second, the national higher educational systems differ in
these two cultures. For example, in Japan, approximately 50% of high
school students enter a university after graduating high school, whereas
Austria has a more flexible higher education system. This difference is
reflected in the significant difference between the participants’ mean
ages, but these small age differences did not influence our results.
Third, some limitations might be a result of different effects and the
importance of social support. The work of Kim et al. reported that
Asians and Asian-Americans are more reluctant to ask for support from
others, but are more likely to use and benefit from forms of support
that do not involve explicit disclosure [40].
Japanese female students’ social support from family, friends and
significant others was highly negatively correlated with stress, as with
SOC, while in Austrians, this relationship was missing, although they
endorsed receiving more social support. Another aspect contributing to
the cultural differences might be response style differences between
Japanese and Austrian students. The work of Harzing et al. showed that
Asians preferred middle rather than extreme categories on rating scales,
in comparison with Western respondents [41]. Using a coherent sample and harmonized measurements in both German and Japanese may have reduced this bias.
Despite
these limitations, we conclude that SOC, BIS and stress are essential
factors that regulate eating behavior in Japan and Austria, with a
relationship to body weight. However, the relationships showed specific
patterns. The relationships between SOC and eating practices in Austrian
and Japanese populations must be further examined. Exploring these
mechanisms will be relevant to fostering the development of programs to
change eating behaviors, because culture is an important external
resource for health promotion.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)