Tuesday, March 7, 2023
Deceptive affection is strategically expressed under relational threat—but not towards partners with low mate value
Rather than generating the filter bubbles that pundits keep hallucinating, googling for political information drives individuals toward sources that are different to their routinary visits
Search engine effects on news consumption: Ranking and representativeness outweigh familiarity in news selection. Roberto Ulloa and Celina Sylwia Kacperski. New Media & Society, Mar 6 2023. https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448231154926
Abstract: While individuals’ trust in search engine results is well-supported, little is known about their preferences when selecting news. We use web-tracked behavioral data across a 2-month period (280 participants) and we analyze three competing factors, two algorithmic (ranking and representativeness) and one psychological (familiarity), that could influence the selection of search results. We use news engagement as a proxy for familiarity and investigate news articles presented on Google search pages (n = 1221). We find a significant effect of algorithmic factors but not of familiarity. We find that ranking plays a lesser role for news compared to non-news, suggesting a more careful decision-making process. We confirm that Google Search drives individuals to unfamiliar sources, and find that it increases the diversity of the political audience of news sources. We tackle the challenge of measuring social science theories in contexts shaped by algorithms, demonstrating their leverage over the behaviors of individuals.
Discussion
Limitations and future directions
Monday, March 6, 2023
The use of social media only stirs up political anger in people who strongly embrace like-minded content, while even heavy users without such blinders become less angry
Birds of a Feather Get Angrier Together: Social Media News Use and Social Media Political Homophily as Antecedents of Political Anger. Zicheng Cheng, Hugo Marcos-Marne & Homero Gil de Zúñiga. Political Behavior, Mar 6 2023. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11109-023-09864-z
Abstract: A significant body of literature within political communication revolves around the constructive political virtues and blighting social and democratic consequences of political anger. For the most part, studies have focused on identifying the primary causes and antecedents of political anger. However, within the context of social media, fewer efforts have been devoted to clarifying how and what infuriates people about politics. Does social media news use relate to increased or reduced levels of political anger? Do social media political homophilic networks explain political anger? And to what extent does political homophily influence the potential effect of social media news use on citizens’ political anger levels—moderating effect? Results drawing on a two-wave U.S. survey dataset show that the frequency of social media news use alone has no direct effect on people’s increased political anger, whereas interacting in homophilic discussion and information networks on social media positively associates with anger. Furthermore, the relationship between social media news use and political anger is contingent upon social media political homophily. Those who report high levels of social media news use and very low levels of social media political homophily end up being less angry over time. Limitations and steps for future research are discussed in the manuscript.
Discussion
Anger is an important political emotion that holds an ambivalent relationship with democracy. While it may serve the powerless to question the political order (Lyman, 2004) and trigger constructive politics before conflicts escalate (Tagar et al., 2011), political anger is also associated with biased assimilation, fueling ideological bias in the acceptance of political information that aligns with one’s opinion (Weeks, 2015). Anger also relates to reliance on pre-existing heuristics and stereotypes (Suhay & Erisen, 2018), increased incivility, hostility, and distrust (Hasell & Weeks, 2016), less willingness to compromise (Mackuen et al., 2010; Wollebæk et al., 2019), and even with violence (Claassen, 2016). Acknowledging the wide range of consequences associated with political anger, this study focused on its social media roots.
Our analysis shows the importance of considering social media political homophily to understand political anger in online environments. Higher levels of social media political homophily do not only associate with political anger, but they also moderate the oftentimes empirically elusive relationship between social media news use and political anger. While social media news use does not contribute to explaining political anger directly, individuals who rank low on social media political homophily will be less angry about politics the more they use social media to consume information about public affairs. More importantly, the direct and moderating effect of social media political homophily on political anger is consistent across all models tested in this study: cross-sectional, lagged, and autoregressive.
Plenty of research has considered whether social media use would produce more homogeneous (Adamic & Glance, 2005; Conover et al., 2011; Feller et al., 2011) or heterogeneous (Choi & Lee, 2015; Kim, 2011; Lee et al., 2014) online environments. Our findings suggest that these studies are of the highest importance to unravel the association between social media news use and political anger. Our findings also suggest that there is no unified answer to social media news. That is, it is not solely about whether people use social media for news or not, but rather other political and communicative predispositions making individuals connect more often with like-minded people and expose themselves to ideologically congruent news. Then, they will be more likely to ‘fall victim’ to the hyper-partisan news and discussion environments which are featured with blame-attribution, moralization, and identity politics framing (Barberá, 2020; Hameleers et al., 2018; Rydell et al., 2008), thus eliciting negative effects like anger. In other words, how people consume social media and curate their news feed exerts an influence on political anger.
Our results demonstrate that whether social media news use is associated with political anger is contingent upon how the specific informational and discussion affordances that social media also provide, more specifically, whether they actively and purposively curate homophilic or heterogeneous social media news and discussion networks. Cinelli et al. (2021) suggested that aggregation of homophilic users dominates the interaction dynamics on social media like Facebook and users tend to seek information that is consistent with his/her preexisting opinion and favor the interaction with like-minded peers, and this situation leads to the formation of polarized groups online. Alternatively, Dubois and Blank (2018) found that a diverse media diet, including news use on multiple media outlets, will direct social media news users toward more diverse information and perspectives, reducing the likelihood of getting into the echo chamber. Results by Guess (2021) are particularly important in this regard, as he demonstrates that most people, at least in the U.S., interact in relatively heterogeneous environments online. While our paper remains agnostic as to the extent to which online homophily is present, the main results provide support for the idea that social media political homophily, when present, matters not only in the context of creating political segregation (Conover et al., 2011), spreading misinformation (Del Vicario et al., 2016), or strengthening group identity (Yardi & boyd, 2010), but also in explaining political anger.
This study adds some nuance to the understanding of social media news users by connecting social media political homophily, social media news use, and political anger. Prior research has suggested that affective polarization is on the rise in the U.S. and that some predicting factors, such as selective exposure (Levendusky, 2013; Tsfati & Nir, 2017) and negative political news coverage (Schmuck et al., 2020), may explain that trend. Our study contributes to filling the research gap by looking into the link between social media use patterns and people’s emotional responses to politics (i.e., political anger). Drawing on our findings, promoting a more heterogeneous social media news use and discussion network can provide an alternative pathway to reducing political anger in the American public. Although political anger has been found to mobilize the public and stimulate political actions, it increases incivility and hostility (Hasell & Weeks, 2016), causes political violence (Claassen, 2016), and exacerbates partisanship and political polarization (Huber et al., 2015). Our results suggest that by altering how individuals engage with social media news people may become less angry, which may trigger a subsequent array of democratically beneficial outcomes such as political tolerance, and less political dogmatism, as well as affect changes in people’s political behavior (Rathnayake & Winter, 2017). From the policy-making perspective, reducing homophily in people’s social and informational networks also seems to be the key. Social media platforms shall address the disadvantages brought by algorithmic news personalization, and efforts should be made to provide social media users with diverse information content, encourage users to follow accounts with opposing views, and interact with peers and news sources that encompass dissimilar political beliefs.
Albeit important, the study is not immune to limitations. There are several shortcomings that must be acknowledged and that might ideally serve as an orientation for further research on the relationship between social media news use, social media political homophily, and political anger. First, while political anger is widely spread across countries, our survey data was collected in a single country, the U.S., and even though it is a panel dataset, it is based only on one year, 2019. In this sense, while the autoregressive models have shown that overall levels of political anger vary to a moderate extent in four months, it is important to see whether choosing a different time lag will make our variables of interest gain more importance (Eveland & Morey, 2011). We also encourage future studies to examine how the link between anger and political behavior may differ across racial groups (Phoenix, 2019), as this study controls for a non-granular white versus minorities dichotomy, and further racial effect nuances may be possible (Magee & Louie, 2016).
Similarly, additional works, comparative in nature, may shed light on the existence of different patterns between social media political homophily and political anger with various degrees of overall anger in the country. Macro, meso, and individual measurement instruments may prove useful here. For instance, there might be country-level moderators for the relationship identified such as the country’s economic condition (Rico et al., 2020), ethnically located injustice (Holmes, 2004), and/or sexism culture (Kay, 2019). Additionally, we measured social media news use and social media political homophily with a self-report survey, which is subject to recall bias and social desirability bias (Scharkow, 2016). As computational methods are increasingly integrated into political communication research, it would be interesting to measure social media news use and social media political homophily with behavioral tracking data. While future studies can adopt more unobstructive measures to palliate potential bias, recent research consistently shows that although self-reported and tracking data on social media news use encompass discrepancies, overall, they positively correlate (Ernala et al., 2020; Haenschen, 2020), which minimizes the impact of this limitation. Besides, it is worth noting that the behavioral tracking data is also subject to measurement error due to the variations of the operating system setting (Jones-Jang et al., 2020), which yields a caveat for using the tracking data as an objective benchmark. As suggested by Jürgens et al. (2020), future work can address the methodological challenge by developing a more advanced digital trace tracking tool, using source-and-issue-specific survey questions, employing longitudinal survey designs (which is done in our study), and combining different data sources. Overall, this study helps clarify the informational and network discussion antecedents of political anger, in a modest but much-needed empirical assessment for the field.
Sunday, March 5, 2023
Severe Sexual Sadism scores were positively associated with strategic emotional intelligence (the ability to understand and manage emotions), but were not significantly related to Experiential EI
Emotional intelligence in incarcerated sexual offenders with sexual sadism. Daniella N. Greenfield et al. Journal of Sexual Aggression, Volume 29, 2023 - Issue 1. Dec 22 2021. https://doi.org/10.1080/13552600.2021.2015469
Abstract: Emotional intelligence (EI) is defined by the ability to perceive, manage, and reason about emotions in oneself and others. Studies have reported deficits in EI abilities among certain antisocial populations such as individuals with psychopathy, and enhanced performance among sexual offenders. Despite EI's relevance to offending behaviour, the association between EI and paraphilic offending has been under-studied. We examined the association between EI, sexual offending, and sexual sadism in 80 incarcerated men with sexual offences and 207 incarcerated men with non-sexual offences. EI was assessed using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Sadism was measured using the Severe Sexual Sadism Scale (SeSaS). Results showed that SeSaS scores were positively associated with Strategic EI (the ability to understand and manage emotions), but were not significantly related to Experiential EI. This may reflect core characteristics of sexual sadism including domination and manipulation, challenging the prevalent notion that higher EI is invariably positive.
Practice impact statement: The study findings suggest that high EI may not always promote prosocial behaviour, which has significant implications for clinical practice. For example, treatment programmes aimed at generally improving EI-related abilities (e.g. emotion regulation) could be refined towards more specific or individualised strategies (e.g. how to effectively and prosocially use EI skills).
Saturday, March 4, 2023
Sexual conflict can arise when males evolve traits that improve their mating success but in doing so harm females; by reducing female fitness, male harm can diminish offspring production in a population and even drive extinction
Male harm offsets the demographic benefits of good genes. Ewan O. Flintham, Vincent Savolainen, and Charles Mullon. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, March 2, 2023, 120 (10) e2211668120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2211668120
Significance: Organisms vary in their biological condition. Those individuals in better condition show improved survival or fecundity, and so populations composed of such individuals should be more viable. However, high-condition individuals also express larger sexually selected phenotypes, some of which, in males, can harm females. The good genes hypothesis posits that sexual selection on condition-dependent traits indirectly increases mean condition and therefore population health. Here, using mathematical models, we show that this effect should rarely be expected when sexual traits cause harm: Instead, good genes selection leads to larger harming traits, reduced female fecundity, and potential population crashes.
Abstract: Sexual conflict can arise when males evolve traits that improve their mating success but in doing so harm females. By reducing female fitness, male harm can diminish offspring production in a population and even drive extinction. Current theory on harm is based on the assumption that an individual’s phenotype is solely determined by its genotype. But the expression of most sexually selected traits is also influenced by variation in biological condition (condition-dependent expression), such that individuals in better condition can express more extreme phenotypes. Here, we developed demographically explicit models of sexual conflict evolution where individuals vary in their condition. Because condition-dependent expression readily evolves for traits underlying sexual conflict, we show that conflict is more intense in populations where individuals are in better condition. Such intensified conflict reduces mean fitness and can thus generate a negative association between condition and population size. The impact of condition on demography is especially likely to be detrimental when the genetic basis of condition coevolves with sexual conflict. This occurs because sexual selection favors alleles that improve condition (the so-called good genes effect), producing feedback between condition and sexual conflict that drives the evolution of intense male harm. Our results indicate that in presence of male harm, the good genes effect in fact easily becomes detrimental to populations.
Discussion
The acute effects of alcohol on social cognition: High alcohol doses tend to have negative effects, low doses may have some positive effects, e.g., on emotion recognition
The acute effects of alcohol on social cognition: A systematic review of experimental studies. Isabelle Cristina Baltariu et al. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, March 2 2023, 109830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2023.109830
Highlights
• This systematic review focused on social cognition changes after drinking alcohol.
• High alcohol doses (>0.8 g alcohol/kg body weight) tend to have negative effects.
• Low doses (<0.4 g/kg) may have some positive effects, e.g., on emotion recognition.
• Interpersonal characteristics may mediate the effects but remain understudied.
Abstract
Rationale: Alcohol effects on social cognition have been studied by measuring facial emotion recognition, empathy, Theory of Mind (ToM) and other forms of information processing.
Objectives: Using the PRISMA guidelines, we reviewed experimental studies that examined effects of alcohol on social cognition.
Methods: Scopus, PsycInfo, PubMed, and Embase were searched between July 2020 - January 2023. The PICO strategy was used for identifying participants, interventions, comparators, and outcomes. Participants (N=2330) were adult social alcohol users. Interventions consisted of acute alcohol administration. Comparators included placebo or the lowest alcohol dose. Outcome variables were grouped into three themes: facial processing, empathy and ToM, and perceptions of inappropriate sexual behavior.
Results: A total of 32 studies were reviewed. Studies measuring facial processing (67%) often found no effects of alcohol on the recognition of specific emotions, effects facilitated emotion recognition at lower doses but worsened emotion recognition at higher doses. In studies measuring empathy or ToM (24%), lower doses were more likely to lead to improvements while higher doses were generally impairing. Within the third group of studies (9%), moderate to high alcohol doses made it more difficult to perceive sexual aggression accurately.
Conclusions: Lower alcohol doses might sometimes help facilitate social cognition, but most data were in line with the idea that alcohol tends to worsen social cognition, particularly at higher doses. Future studies might focus on examining other moderators of the effects of alcohol on social cognition, particularly interpersonal characteristics such as trait emotional empathy and participant and target gender.
Keywords: alcoholsocial cognitionemotion recognitionempathytheory of mindaggression
4. Discussion
This systematic review summarized experimental studies on alcohol’s acute effects on social cognition, with the aim to gain a better understanding of alcohol-induced changes in social cognition as a mediator in the alcohol - aggression relationship. While people often drink with others to obtain positive interpersonal outcomes (Peele & Brodsky, 2000), changes in the processing of social stimuli may also have inadvertent negative consequences, at least in some people. To examine social cognition changes after drinking, we inspected past alcohol administration studies that assessed alcohol’s effects on facial processing (theme 1), empathy and ToM (theme 2), and perceptions of inappropriate sexual behavior (theme 3). We reviewed 32 studies involving mostly healthy adult volunteers. Methodologically, studies used a wide variety of alcohol doses (range 0.14-2.5 g/kg) and outcome measures.
With respect to theme 1, one key finding of our systematic review was that alcohol can increase the accurate recognition of happiness in emotional faces at both low and high doses (Dolder et al., 2017, Kano et al., 2003, Nagar et al., 2021, Sripada et al., 2011) while at moderate and high doses it can decrease the recognition of fear, sadness, and anger (Attwood et al., 2009a, Attwood et al., 2009b, Borrill et al., 1987, Craig et al., 2009, Dolder et al., 2017, Eastwood et al., 2020, Honan et al., 2018, Kano et al., 2003, Khouja et al., 2019, Nagar et al., 2021, Sripada et al., 2011, Stevens et al., 2008, Stevens et al., 2009). Different emotions are thought to communicate different intentions, with anger inviting confrontation and fear inviting protection, for example (Parkinson, 1996). On the one hand, this has been interpreted as alcohol inducing positive cognitive biases. On the other hand, it is possible that alcohol consumption impairs one’s sensitivity to signals of submission and distress from others, thus increasing the probability of responding aggressively (Attwood & Munafò, 2014).
The results for disgust recognition were less consistent, with both decreases and increases at moderate to high doses (Attwood et al., 2009a, Borrill et al., 1987, Eastwood et al., 2020, Felisberti and Terry, 2015). While alcohol does not always have effects on facial emotion recognition, this does not appear directly related to the administered alcohol dose, which implies that other factors may moderate the effects. For instance, gender (Craig et al., 2008), social anxiety (Stevens et al., 2009), and trait emotional empathy (Dolder et al., 2017) may have a moderating influence. The observed effects of alcohol on facial emotion recognition suggest that, in most individuals, alcohol increases social information processing in a way that may improve their mood (Dolder et al., 2017, Kano et al., 2003, Sripada et al., 2011) while particularly at higher doses the effects on social cognition may be more likely to induce a negative emotional state (Craig et al., 2008; Eastwood et al., 2020; Honan et al., 2018) thereby explaining a higher risk for aggression (Heinz et al., 2011). Besides impairing detection of negative expressions, drinking alcohol can increase the misclassification of negative expressions as neutral (Atwood et al., 2009a; Kamboj et al., 2013). While this may serve to reduce social anxiety (Stevens et al., 2008), thereby making alcohol a social lubricant, at the same time alcohol might reduce controlled, effortful cognitive processing, thereby creating a narrowing effect on attention. In this process, called “alcohol myopia”, the range of social cues that can be adequately processed becomes restricted (Steele & Josephs, 1990). Even though alcohol can have an anxiety- reducing effect at lower doses, at higher doses can negatively influence social judgment (Francis et al., 2019, Johnson et al., 2018, Herzog, 1999).
A second key finding for theme 1 was that while at moderate doses alcohol can increase facial scanning and improve facial memory (Colloff & Flowe; 2016) at high and moderate doses these cognitive processes are more likely to become disrupted. In general, studies were more likely to find positive effects on social information processing at lower doses and to find impairing effects at higher alcohol doses (Borrill et al., 1987, Harvey, 2014, Sripada et al., 2011). These findings are in line with the idea that dose moderates the effects of alcohol on social cognition. For example the findings in the study by Penton-Voak et al. (2012) suggest that after a moderate alcohol dose women perceive a threat in men but they can have increased sex-related cognitions; while men get worse at perceiving social signals this leading to escalating male-male aggression.
With respect to theme 2, while studies found that moderate doses had variable effects, high doses generally decreased empathy and ToM (Battista et. al., 2014; Francis et al., 2019; Hu et al., 2018; Karlén et al., 2019). The decrease in empathy may be closely linked to impaired recognition of negative emotions (particularly fear and sadness) as mentioned above. Moreover, decreased empathic ability has been linked to aggression before (Dethier and Blairy, 2012, Philippot et al., 2005).
Finally, three studies that focused on perceptions of inappropriate sexual behavior (theme 3) all found that, at moderate to high doses, alcohol negatively influences the accurate processing of cues of sexual aggression (Gross et al., 2001, Loiselle and Fuqua, 2007). These results could be taken to mean that alcohol increases the risk of sexual aggression. This idea is consistent with multiple previous studies that found increased hostility and aggressive behavior after alcohol, even though these studies generally did not focus on sexual aggression (Bushman and Cooper, 1990, Ogle and Miller, 2004). Indeed, while all reviewed studies focused on aggression between men and women, experimental studies on the effect of alcohol on aggression have generally not examined both participant gender and target gender.
The results for alcohol’s effects on the perception of inappropriate sexual behavior can be connected to the aforementioned results on facial emotion processing (theme 1): the reduced anger recognition accuracy (Khouja et al., 2019) and increased tendency to see happy expressions (Dolder et al., 2017, Khouja et al., 2019) could help explain why, after moderate doses of alcohol, women had worse risk detection while listening to a date rape (Loiselle & Fuqua, 2007). Similarly, men’s tendency to judge women’s friendliness and arousal as higher after a moderate or high alcohol doses may be due to impaired processing of sad, disgusted, and fearful (Felisberti and Terry, 2015, Honan et al., 2018, Kamboj et al., 2013) expressions, in combination with improved processing of happy expressions (Dolder et al., 2017, Kano et al., 2003, Sripada et al., 2011). Moreover, men’s decreased perception of antisocial behavior at high alcohol doses (Karlén et al., 2019) fits with the results about decreased anger recognition (Borrill et al., 1987, Khouja et al., 2019). Importantly, women but not men became better at perceiving antisocial behavior, both with the moderate alcohol dose and with the high dose (Karlén et al., 2019). These findings are in line with previous studies that confirmed the alcohol-aggression link: experimental studies showed that acute alcohol consumption can lead to aggression (Chermack and Taylor, 1995, Dougherty et al., 1999, Giancola, 2002, Giancola, 2004, Zeichner et al., 1994), while correlational studies found that even in the context of chronic alcohol use aggression is most often reported during episodes of acute intoxication (Chermack & Giancola, 1997). In line with our results, clinical evidence suggests that emotion recognition and ToM ability is indeed impaired in AUD individuals (Kumar et al., 2022, Le Berre, 2019), and this is thought to be due to the large amount of drinking.
Overall, our review suggests that although alcohol in lower doses may improve social functioning, at higher doses alcohol is particularly likely to impair social cognition, thereby it may increase the risk of aggression in some people. Specifically, while results were not consistent across studies, lower doses were able to increase facial scanning (Colloff and Flowe, 2016, Penton-Voak et al., 2012), happiness recognition (Dolder et al., 2017, Kano et al., 2003), empathy and ToM (Dolder et al., 2017, Johnson et al., 2018), and decrease the recognition of negative emotions (Atwood et al., 2009b; Craig et al., 2008; Eastwood et al., 2020; Honan et al., 2018; Kamboj et al., 2013; Nagar et al., 2021). Higher alcohol doses were able to decrease not only the recognition of negative emotions (Sripada et al., 2011) but also facial scanning (Colloff and Flowe, 2016, Honan et al., 2018), and happiness recognition (Kano et al., 2003), empathy and ToM (Francis et al., 2019, Herzog, 1999, Thiel et al., 2018). The fact that results were not consistent across studies might partially be due to moderating factors other than alcohol dose. Individual characteristics such as participant gender (Battista et al., 2014; Craig et al., 2008; Karlén et al., 2019, Penton-Voak et al., 2012) and social anxiety (Stevens et al., 2008, Stevens et al., 2009) may play a role, but so may situational variables such as target gender (Penton-Voak et. al., 2012) and social context (e.g., Dolder et al., 2017; Thiel et al., 2018). For one, we propose that more careful studies of (participant and target) gender as a potential moderator of the effects of alcohol on social cognition are needed. Importantly, researchers conducting these studies should decide on whether to vary doses by participant gender or not. Additionally, we propose to study relevant interpersonal characteristics as potential moderators, such as trait emotional empathy (Dolder et al., 2017, Giancola, 2003).
4.1. Limitations of the review
We previously noted that the studies reviewed in this manuscript used a wide variety of alcohol doses (range 0.14-2.5 g/kg). While categorizing alcohol doses into low, moderate, and high facilitates their interpretation, this categorization could lead to missing subtle differences between doses, as BAC would more accurately represent alcohol levels in the blood (Martin and Sayette, 1993). However, only two of the reviewed studies (Kamboj et al., 2013, Nagar et al., 2021) compared alcohol’s effects at lower and higher BAC.
Related, we also noted that the experimental procedures for alcohol administration differed substantially between studies. In this context, it is of relevance that alcohol has biphasic effects: stimulation is observed on the ascending limb of the absorption curve and sedation is observed on the descending limb (Pihl et al., 2003). To get to the peak of the ascending limb it takes 36 minutes and to get to the peak of the descending limb 109 minutes (Marczinski & Fillmore, 2005). In some studies, participants were asked to start completing performance measures of social cognition immediately after alcohol consumption (Francis et al., 2019, Stevens et al., 2009, Thiel et al., 2018), while in another study, participants were instructed to wait 40 minutes while completing baseline questionnaires (Kamboj et al., 2013) or to complete the task only when the desired level of intoxication was achieved (assessed using breathalyzer tests) (Colloff & Flowe, 2016). Participants in the former studies were thus more likely to complete tasks while on the ascending limb than participants in the latter studies. This between-study variation in the time between alcohol administration and social cognition assessment might help explain between-study variation in the effects of alcohol on facial emotion processing (theme 1), for example. In addition, not all facial emotion recognition studies report an overall alcohol effect on general emotion recognition, however in which studies this information was available, this effect also varied from small (Khojua et al., 2019; Padula et al., 2011), moderate (Borrill et al., 1987;Craig et al., 2008; Eastwood et al., 2020; Felisberti & Terry, 2015; Tucker & Vuchinich, 1983) and high (Honan et al., 2017; Nagar et al., 2021; Sripada et al., 2011).
Another limitation of our review was that we included blinded studies (involving double or single blinding, see Table 1) (Atwood et al., 2009a; Atwood et al., 2009b; Craig et al., 2008; Dolder et al., 2017; Felisberti and Terry, 2015; Kano et al., 2003), unblinded studies (Battista et al., 2014, Farris et al., 2010, Herzog, 1999), and studies for which blinding was not reported (Stevens et al., 2008, Stevens et al., 2009). Most of these studies included participants who at least expected to drink alcohol during the study. People with alcohol expectations tend to adapt their behavior to match expected alcohol effects (Martin and Sayette, 1993). Therefore, the use of an effective placebo manipulation is generally recommended (Babor, 2000) but our review included studies that differed in the way the blinding was organized and if it was successful. Also, studies did not report information on the blinding of researchers, and if this was checked.
A final limitation of our review is that the measures used to assess aspects of social cognition variables varied widely across studies. Some of the reviewed tasks offer more ecological validity in the assessment of social cognition thanks to the stimuli having similarities with real life situations during which a person might consider others’ emotions, thoughts, intentions, or behaviors (Hogenelst et al., 2015). Examples include the empathic accuracy task, or the video tape of interpersonal violence situation, and even the audiotape of the date rape. Conversely, results from basic tasks involving simple stimuli, e.g. black and white still images of facial expressions should be interpreted in light of their relative lack of ecological validity.
4.2. Implications of this review for future research
The present review shows that drinking alcohol can negatively affect social cognition, particularly at higher doses. This supports the idea that changes in social cognition can mediate alcohol’s effect on aggression. However, given substantial variability in results between studies, even at comparable (low-moderate-high) doses, we recommend that future studies focus on elucidating which person-level and situation-level variables might act as moderators. For example, at the person level, factors like alcohol tolerance, metacognitive processes, personality factors (Kalant, 1996, Kuntsche et al., 2008, Spada and Wells, 2006) might be interesting to study because the alcohol aggression relationship may not be present all the time and in all individuals. Also, situational aspects of a social interaction involving alcohol (or an interaction-like situation created in a performance-based measure) might be considered. For example, the next step would be studying the behavior in a social interaction situation (Zawacki, 2011) or employing several potential relevant social cognition measures after drinking (Schreiter et al., 2013).
Finally, the finding that alcohol can acutely impair social cognition, particularly at higher alcohol doses (this review) does not exclude the possibility of reverse causality. That is, by improving social cognition, social drinking may be reduced. In other words, social cognition training may diminish alcohol use in social settings, which may have clinical implications. Several such training options are available: micro-expressions training (Russell et al., 2006), example emotion and ToM imitation training (Mazza et al., 2010), and a dynamic interactive social cognition training in virtual reality (Nijman et al., 2020). While the effects of social cognition training on drinking in a social context have not been studied, to learn more about the role of social cognition in the alcohol-aggression link, this may be an important next step.